Losing smarts through your smartphone: Multimedia use and achievement

The TV is tuned to the Superbowl channel and popcorn and a cold beverage are at hand. What more could one need to enjoy a few relaxing hours? Actually, if you are among the people who media multitask, then you would have to throw in at least a smartphone and a laptop to satisfy your craving for more media (a heartrate monitor, aquarium, and blender are optional extras).

Source: Flickr Creative Commons

Media multitasking has become increasingly common, as TIME magazine already pointed out in 2011. Most parents of contemporary teenagers are familiar with daily disputations about whether homework and TV are incompatible constructs or harmonious complements of each other.

Unsurprisingly, smart media technology has also become a focus of research in cognitive psychology: We have already reported on this site on research that examined how one’s driving skills deteriorate during smartphone use, and we also recently noted that some people find their phones so irresistible, they may not make it through dinner without checking their email or Twitter feed.

But what about situations in which media multitasking is less dangerous than while driving and less socially awkward than during dinner? What about that contemporary way of doing homework, with a TV and a laptop plus smartphone with one earplug dedicated to music, the other one left dangling so the TV has a chance to pipe its messages into the adolescent brain? Is old-fashioned single-channel parenting supported by the data?

recent article in the Psychonomic Bulletin & Review addressed the effects of such media multitasking on children’s academic and cognitive performance.

Researchers Cain, Leonard, Gabrieli, and Finn administered a “Media Use Questionnaire” to a sample of eight-graders living in the greater Boston area. The questionnaire probed how many hours per week children spent watching television or videos, listening to music, playing video games, reading print or electronic media, talking on the phone, using instant or text messaging, or creating crafts or writing.

 

In addition, participants indicated how often they combined each of these activities with another one. To separate the extent of multitasking from the amount of consumption of media, the authors computed a Media-Multitasking Index (MMI) that ranged from 0 (all media consumed singly) to 12 (all media consumed simultaneously with all other media) but was unaffected by the total amount consumed. For example, a child who played video games 40 hours a week but did nothing else at the same time (except perhaps the occasional glance at the homework) might still have a low MMI.

Participants reported consuming a great deal of media, with the TV (surprisingly?) still being the primary channel of consumption. The MMI scores averaged out to around 3.0, representing 25% concurrent media usage. However, the distribution had a positive skew, with a fairly fat upper tail extending into the 50% region and beyond.

In order to examine how media multitasking affects cognitive performance, Cain and colleagues also obtained indicators of academic performance (namely, Math and English Language Arts scores maintained by the Massachusetts Department of Elementary and Secondary Education) and tested participants on various cognitive tasks and attributes, such as their working memory and cognitive processing speed, their manual dexterity and vocabulary. In addition, participants’ levels of “grit” (i.e., their ability to persevere in difficult or even impossible tasks), conscientiousness and impulsiveness were also tested.

The results were discouraging for multi-media users and confirming the single-channel parents’ intuitions: In a nutshell, the more students multi-tasked outside of school, the worse they fared on both components of their test scores (math and English language arts). This effect was of medium size, with a correlation coefficient of -.3, implying that just under 10% of the total variance in students’ scores was accounted for by their multi-media index (MMI).

Of course, this relationship might be due to a variety of factors and the direction of causality is unclear. For example, poor students might be so disappointed by their performance at school that they resort to more media for gratification, or there might be yet another variable explaining the association.

To narrow down these possibilities, Cain and colleagues explored how performance on the various cognitive tasks related to the MMI.

Cain and colleagues found that the MMI was negatively associated with two measures of working memory, acounting span and an N-back task. By contrast, several measures of crystallized intelligence, namely vocabulary, comprehension and calculation performance, were not associated with the MMI. The selectivity of those associations suggests that multi-media use does not impair cognition generally, but it may have an adverse impact on those tasks that require attentional and executive control.

This interpretation was supported by the further fact the MMI was associated with impulsivity, that is children’s tendency to act on a whim, displaying behavior characterized by little or no forethought, reflection, or consideration of the consequences. None of the other personality attributes considered (e.g., grit or conscientiousness) were associated with multi-media use, again suggesting that the effect of media multi-tasking is quite selective.

In summary, the results of Cain and colleagues suggest that only tasks and behavioral attributes that involve attentional control are negatively associated with multi-media use. Notably, those associations arise at a young age, before adulthood, and they can be picked up in “real life” measures that matter a great deal to a person’s life—namely their scholastic performance.

What are the practical implications of those results?

At first glance, as a parent one might be tempted to regulate the amount of time that one’s teenagers spend watching television while also playing video games or using their phones. However, even setting aside the obvious difficulties of actually doing this, such attempts at regulation might be premature: As one of the authors, Amy Finn, put it: “The direction of causality is difficult to establish. For example, media multitasking may be a consequence of underlying cognitive differences and not vice versa.” Even though the specificity of the associations is compatible with a causal interpretation, the data do not permit any unambiguous inference for now.

Article focused on in this post:

Cain, M.S. et al. (2016). Media multitasking in adolescence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, DOI:10.3758/s13423-016-1036-3.

Author

  • Stephan Lewandowsky

    Stephan Lewandowsky's research examines memory, decision making, and knowledge structures, with a particular emphasis on how people update information in memory. He has also contributed nearly 50 opinion pieces to the global media on issues related to climate change "skepticism" and the coverage of science in the media.

    View all posts

The Psychonomic Society (Society) is providing information in the Featured Content section of its website as a benefit and service in furtherance of the Society’s nonprofit and tax-exempt status. The Society does not exert editorial control over such materials, and any opinions expressed in the Featured Content articles are solely those of the individual authors and do not necessarily reflect the opinions or policies of the Society. The Society does not guarantee the accuracy of the content contained in the Featured Content portion of the website and specifically disclaims any and all liability for any claims or damages that result from reliance on such content by third parties.

You may also like